Primates
© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer-Verlag 2006
10.1007/s10329-006-0195-7

Original Article

Survey of Hylobates agilis albibarbis in a logged peat-swamp forest: Sabangau catchment, Central Kalimantan

Cara Buckley1, 2, K. A. I. NekarisContact Information and Simon John Husson2, 3

(1)  Department of Anthropology, Centre for Conservation, Environment and Development, School of Social Sciences and Law, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, OX3 0BP, UK
(2)  The Orangutan Tropical Peatland Research Project, CIMTROP, University of Palangkaraya, Palangkaraya, Central Kalimantan, Indonesia
(3)  Wildlife Research Group, Department of Anatomy, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, UK

Contact Information K. A. I. Nekaris
Email: anekaris@brookes.ac.uk
Phone: +44-1865-483767

Received: 16 June 2005  Accepted: 28 March 2006  Published online: 31 May 2006

Abstract  Few data are available on gibbon populations in peat-swamp forest. In order to assess the importance of this habitat for gibbon conservation, a population of Hylobates agilis albibarbis was surveyed in the Sabangau peat-swamp forest, Central Kalimantan, Indonesia. This is an area of about 5,500 km2 of selectively logged peat-swamp forest, which was formally gazetted as a national park during 2005. The study was conducted during June and July 2004 using auditory sampling methods. Five sample areas were selected and each was surveyed for four consecutive days by three teams of researchers at designated listening posts. Researchers recorded compass bearings of, and estimated distances to, singing groups. Nineteen groups were located. Population density is estimated to be 2.16 (±0.46) groups/km2. Sightings occurring either at the listening posts or that were obtained by tracking in on calling groups yielded a mean group size of 3.4 individuals, hence individual gibbon density is estimated to be 7.4 (±1.59) individuals/km2. The density estimates fall at the mid-range of those calculated for other gibbon populations, thus suggesting that peat-swamp forest is an important habitat for gibbon conservation in Borneo. A tentative extrapolation of results suggests a potential gibbon population size of 19,000 individuals within the mixed-swamp forest habitat sub-type in the Sabangau. This represents one of the largest remaining continuous populations of Bornean agile gibbons. The designation of the Sabangau forest as a national park will hopefully address the problem of illegal logging and hunting in the region. Further studies should note any difference in gibbon density post protection.

Keywords  Auditory sampling - Bornean agile gibbon - Peat-swamp forest - Population density - Sabangau catchment


Introduction

The ability to survey primate populations is a vital conservation tool, especially for assessing populations at the initial phase (National Research Council 1981; Brockelman and Ali 1987; Sutherland 2002). Although gibbons have formed the focus of many behavioural and ecological studies, limited data are available in terms of their population and demographic trends (Mitani 1990; OrsquoBrien et al. 2003, 2004), due to low visibility and unreliable behaviour upon detection (Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993; Nijman 2004).

Twelve species of gibbon comprise at least 29 taxa (Geissmann 2002–2003). Three subspecies of agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis) are recognised: the mountain agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis albibarbis) found in West Sumatra; the lowland agile gibbon (H.a. unko) found in East Sumatra and Peninsular Malaysia; and the Bornean agile gibbon (H.a. albibarbis) found in Indonesian Borneo (Geissmann 2000). H.a. albibarbis is endemic to Borneo, and is found in south-west Kalimantan between the Kapuas and Barito Rivers (Marshall and Sugardjito 1986; Geissmann 2000; Brandon-Jones et al. 2004). All three subspecies of agile gibbon are listed as IUCN Lower Risk: NT (Eudey 2000).

Gibbons are largely frugivorous, arboreal apes that live in small groups of two to six individuals (Gittins and Raemaekers 1980; Leighton 1987; OrsquoBrien et al. 2003). They occupy home ranges that vary between 7 and 58 ha in size, and which may include territories that are more or less exclusive (e.g. Gittins and Raemaekers 1980; Leighton 1987; Mitani 1990; McConkey et al. 2003). Gibbons utter characteristic calls most mornings, which can be heard over a distance of up to 2 km in some terrain (Gittins and Raemaekers 1980; Reichard and Sommer 1997; OrsquoBrien et al. 2004), although audibility of calls does not generally exceed 1 km through level forest (Brockelman and Ali 1987).

Due to the vocal nature of gibbons, surveying with auditory sampling methods has proven more successful than line-transect techniques (Wilson and Johns 1982; Johns 1985; Mather 1992; Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993; Nijman 2004). Normally, the duet call is used; a bout of singing given by a mated pair (as defined by Gittins 1984). With reference to H.a. albibarbis, duets typically commence with the male followed by the female, who joins in by giving phrases similar to the male and then produces great calls (Gittins 1984). Duets are distinct from other vocal behaviour (Brockelman and Ali 1987), containing a great call by the female and a lsquocodarsquo by the male (Geissmann 2000).

With increased ability to access remote peat-swamp forests, it has now been shown that they are richer in biodiversity than previously thought (Page et al. 1997). They also perform a range of important environmental functions, including carbon and water storage and climate regulation (Rieley et al. 1997; MacKinnon et al. 1997a; Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2003).

The largest single area of peat-swamp forest remaining in Borneo is located in the catchments of the Sabangau and Bulan Rivers in the south of Central Kalimantan, hereon referred to as the Sabangau catchment. Research on the biodiversity of peat-swamp forest has been ongoing since 1993; this area is now recognised as containing the largest population of wild Bornean orang-utans, Pongo pygmaeus (Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2003; Singleton et al. 2004).

In addition to orang-utans, the Sabangau catchment is host to eight other primate species (Page et al. 1997). Data regarding the status, behaviour and ecology of these taxa are lacking in the Sabangau region. The focus in this study was H.a. albibarbis with the following objectives: to carry out a population density survey; to determine average group size and age/class estimation; to compare results with agile gibbon data from other research sites; to assess the importance of the Sabangau catchment for conservation of this species.


Methods
Site location
The Sabangau catchment forms part of an extensive peat-covered landscape in Central Kalimantan, which stretches from the Seruyan River in the west to the Barito River in the east (Fig. 1). A series of peat domes have formed in the floodplains between major rivers, each forested and largely undeveloped until the mid-1990s, when a combination of macro-scale agricultural projects and forest fires removed much of the forest cover. The Sabangau catchment is the largest (ca. 7,200 km2) and least-developed landscape within this area, with contiguous forest cover of 5,584 km2 (Husson et al. 2006); settlements occur only on the banks of the surrounding rivers. The primary research site is the Laboratorium Alam Hutan Gambut (LAHG; Natural Laboratory for the Study of Peat Swamp Forest) a 500-km2 area gazetted for research and located in the northeast of the catchment (Fig. 1).
MediaObjects/s10329-006-0195-7fmb1.gif
Fig. 1 Map of Borneo showing the location of the Laboratorium Alam Hutan Gambut (LAHG) and the Sabangau catchment. Forest cover is given for land between the Sabangau and Katingan rivers only

Domed peatlands typically exhibit a catenary sequence of habitat sub-types, distributed in relation to peat depth and water table and, hence, distance from the watershed (MacKinnon et al. 1997b; Rieley et al. 1997). Three peat-swamp forest sub-types have been identified: a relatively-diverse mixed-swamp forest (MSF) found at the perimeter of the dome, a lower-canopy, species poor low-pole forest (LPF) near the centre of the swamp, and, occurring only in the Sabangau peat-swamp forest, a species-rich tall-interior forest (TIF), at the watershed of the peat dome (Page et al. 1999). This study was carried out in MSF, this habitat sub-type is relatively high in diversity and has a canopy of about 35 m (Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2003).

The LAHG stands on the site of a disused logging concession that practised selective logging for 30 years. When the concession ended in 1996, illegal loggers moved into the area (Husson et al. 2006). The route of the timber extraction railway still remains, and forms the eastern boundary of a grid system. The 9-km2 grid system is situated in MSF and contains 13 major transects, which are marked with flagging tape every 25 m.

Survey methods

Surveys were carried out from 28 June to 27 July 2004. With level or undulating terrain, how far groups can be heard cannot easily be determined from a topographic map (Brockelman and Ali 1987). Triangulation was thus required. Three listening posts labelled A, B and C were established at each of five localities, and manned by three teams of two researchers for a period of 4 days. These localities were selected within a 4-km2 area: one in the centre and the others in each of the four corners. For the first four localities, spacing of the listening posts exceeded the minimum recommended distance of 300 m (Brockelman and Ali 1987). Due to constraints of the grid system, location five was an exception, with two of the distances between listening posts under 300 m, but this did not alter the effective listening area (Table 2).

Researchers were in position by 0430 hours, and remained at the listening post until 1000 hours or for 30 min after the last group was heard calling. Sightings were recorded when they occurred. Researchers recorded the time, compass direction and estimated distance to all groups heard singing. Synchronised bearings were recorded at intervals of 3 min. Lengths of song bouts were recorded using a stopwatch, and were deemed to have ended if the gibbons were silent for more than 2 min (Gittins 1984).

Frequency of calling

In order to use loud calls for sampling gibbon populations, the average probability of calling, and the variables that affect frequency, must be studied in a population of known groups (Brockelman and Ali 1987; Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993). OrsquoBrien et al. (2004) found that the probability of agile gibbons calling stabilises by day 4. Thus, each listening post was surveyed for a period of four consecutive days during this study, with the exception of the second sample period, which was surveyed from the 6–7 July and 9–10 July. Heavy rain on the morning of 8 July made data collection impossible. Probability of calling rates was generated from the data collected during this study after the research period, based on the number of groups located and the number of calls recorded for each group.

Mapping

Triangulation points and sightings were marked daily on maps of the study area, and ultimately overlaid onto a single map, allowing researchers to estimate the number of groups present during the research period (Estrada et al. 2004). Points mapped more than 500 m apart were considered to be different groups (Brockelman and Ali 1987; Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993; OrsquoBrien et al. 2004). This figure is based on the approximate diameter of a grouprsquos range and determines the maximum distance that agile gibbons might move between calls (OrsquoBrien et al. 2004). A home range of 70 ha would have a radius of 472 m. It was thus felt that 500 m was a conservative separation for calling groups, which incorporated the possibility of larger home ranges for peat-swamp forest gibbons and the ellipsoid nature of many home ranges (OrsquoBrien et al. 2004). Points mapped less than 500 m apart were assigned to groups by establishing which groups were singing simultaneously, supplemented with acoustical and directional information. Only the acoustical points and sightings represent when the position of groups was actually determined.

In addition to recording locations of gibbon calls, researchers attempted to locate groups by sight that were close to the listening stations. Group size and composition (number of individuals of each age/sex class) were recorded when a group was sighted, and the grouprsquos location noted using GPS and added to the map.

Data analysis
Density estimates were obtained by the use of the following formula (Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993):
 $$ D = \frac{n} {E} $$
Where: D=density, n=number of groups heard per sample period, E=effective listening area.
The effective listening area is defined as the zone in which at least two of the three teams could hear calling gibbon groups up to 1 km away (Brockelman and Ali 1987). The effective listening area was calculated for each sample period (Fig. 2). Mean group size and standard deviations were obtained through averaging across sightings and was used to calculate an individual density.
MediaObjects/s10329-006-0195-7fmb2.gif
Fig. 2 How the effective listening area was calculated, using the second sample period as an example. Dark grey proportion of effective listening area where researchers from all three posts could hear gibbon groups up to 1 km away. Light grey proportion of effective listening area where researchers from two posts could hear gibbon groups up to 1 km away. Black proportion of effective listening area where researchers from one post could hear gibbon groups up to 1 km away

Weather

Gibbons seldom sing when it is raining, and windy conditions have also been shown to inhibit singing (Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993; Leighton 1987). Cloud cover and wind level were thus recorded at 10-min intervals (Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993; OrsquoBrien et al. 2004). Temperature was noted daily; rainfall was recorded once in the morning and evening. To ascertain if weather conditions had an effect on singing frequency or start times, product-moment correlation coefficient, Mann–Whitney U, and chi-square tests were used, with P=0.05.


Results
Frequency and probability of calling

Agile gibbons at LAHG called most frequently between 0500 and 0700 hours (73% of calls recorded). Start times prior to 0500 hours accounted for 12% of all calls recorded while 9% of calls occurred between 0700 and 0800 hours. Calls recorded after 0800 hours accounted for 4% of calls and calls recorded after 0900 hours accounted for just 2%. The average probability of a single group calling on any given day was 0.62 (n=20 days). Across sample periods, the probability of a group calling on day 1 was 0.59 (±0.14, n=5 days). The probability of a group calling on at least one of the first 2 days was 0.92 (n=10 days), and of calling on at least one of the first 3 days was 1.0 (n=15 days).

Triangulation of calls and mapping
Researchers recorded 125 calls over a period of 20 days, 29 of which were solo calls. As it is impossible to determine if these calls were made by paired or unpaired males, thus either over or under-estimating population density, they were not used in the density analysis. The remaining 96 were duets of which 65 produced two-point bearings, three-point bearings or triangulation. These were plotted on a map of the grid system to estimate the number of groups (Fig. 3).
MediaObjects/s10329-006-0195-7fmb3.gif
Fig. 3 The location of gibbon (Hylobates agilis albibarbis) groups. The position of all the listening posts are indicated, as is the old railway track that forms the eastern boundary of the grid system. A cross indicates three-point bearings; this was when the compass bearing from each team met at a perfect point. A star indicates two-point bearings; this was when the calling group was only heard by two of the three teams. A triangle indicates where three bearings produced triangulation. This means that the gibbon group in question was calling from somewhere within the triangle. A circle indicates sightings. All bearings and sightings are marked with the time and date. A jagged line marks possible boundaries between groups. The shaded areas indicate which of the acoustical points or sightings were attributed to which group. Those that could not be confidently assigned remain unshaded

Nineteen groups were identified and are referred to as G1–G19. The larger appearance of G13 and G17 is due to the ranges of these groups being closer to the listening posts, subsequently yielding more data. Other groups appeared to have only one edge of their range within the effective listening area and, therefore, a smaller sample size was recorded.

Gibbon sightings
Altogether, eight sightings of nine groups occurred, which are summarised in Table 1. Two group encounters were witnessed; marked on the map as probable boundaries between groups. Two lengthy sightings of G17 and one sighting of G13 confirmed that they were separate groups, as the female of G17 carried an infant while the female of G13 did not. As the sightings of these groups occurred within 200 m of one another, another possible inter-group boundary is shown on the map. The sightings yielded an average group size of 3.4 individuals (n=9 sightings). The average duration of sightings was 21.2 min. All sightings occurred either by tracking in on calling groups or while researchers were stationed at the listening posts.
Table 1 All gibbon (Hylobates agilis albibarbis) sightings during research period (n=8)

Group name

Duration (min)

Total no. of animals seen

Adult male

Adult female

Sub-adult male

Sub-adult female

Adolescent/Juveniles

Infants

G17

27a

5

1

1

 

1

1

1

44a

5

1

1

 

1

1

1

G15

20

4

1

1

   

1

1

G13

14

4

1

1

1

 

1

 

G5

25b

4

1

1

 

1

1

 

G6

25b

3

1

1

   

1

 

G9

22

3

1

1

   

1

 

Unknown

10

4

1

1

 

1

1

 

G10

10b

2

1

1

       

G19

15b

2

1

1

       

Total

 

31

9

9

1

3

8

2

Mean group size

 

3.4

           
Infants still carried by their mothers during travel. Juveniles travelled independently, size less than half that of adults. Adolescents more than one half adult size but not yet fully grown. Sub-adults similar in size to adults but not yet mated (Mitani 1990)
a Sightings seen by two teams and counted once
b Sightings which were encounters between more than one group
Density
The formula yielded an average density estimate of 2.16 groups/km2 or 7.4 individuals/km2 (Table 2). Individual density was generated by multiplying group density by the mean group size stated above. Based on the density estimate, about 47 ha of forest are available for each group, but gaps in the canopy, potential overlap between groups and selective habitat use means that further research is needed to determine home range size.
Table 2 Comparison of gibbon group and individual densities calculated for each sample period with standard deviation

Listening post no.

Effective listening area (km2)

Groups/km2

Individuals/km2

1

3.0

1.67

5.73

2

2.9

1.72

5.93

3

2.9

2.76

9.49

4

3.0

2.33

8.03

5

3.0

2.33

8.03

Mean±SD

 

2.16±0.46

7.4±1.59

Weather

Wind conditions were overwhelmingly calm (98%, n=20) and thus had no effect on calling frequency, or start times. Cloud cover was clear on 35% of days, >50% on 10% of days and <50% on 55% of days (n=20). No significant relationship was found between cloud cover and calling frequency or start times. Minimum temperatures averaged 21°C (range 20–24°C) and maximum temperatures averaged 29°C (range 27–31°C), but had no statistical relationship with calling frequency or start times.

A total of 31.4 cm of rain fell during the research period, which took place during the annual dry season. On the mornings of gibbon surveys, it drizzled slightly once. Rainfall tended to occur during the late afternoon and overnight. Rainfall during the night preceding data collection correlated positively with start times of calling (rp=0.519, n=20, P<0.02) (Fig. 4).
MediaObjects/s10329-006-0195-7flb4.gif
Fig. 4 Figure showing relationship between rainfall the previous night and start times of calling. One can note an increase in start times of calling groups, with a corresponding increase in rainfall


Discussion
Sightings and auditory sampling

For gibbon surveys, researchers more frequently use auditory as opposed to visual cues (but see Alfred and Sati 1990). Haimoff et al. (1986) located 20 groups during a survey of black-crested gibbons (Nomascus concolor) through auditory cues, only six of which were sighted. Similar to our study, the most direct and prolonged sightings took place while the gibbons were calling (Haimoff et al. 1986).

As in other studies of vocal primates (e.g. Gursky 1998; Hanya et al. 2003), our study combined two techniques. Density estimates were obtained through the mapping of calls, and information on groups was obtained by tracking in on calling groups and through opportunistic sightings at the listening posts, where detection was not compromised by the noise of walking on uneven terrain (Ross and Reeve 2003). These techniques were especially successful during our restricted research period. As calls can be heard over greater distances than the animals are detectable by sight, the inclusion of auditory cues increases the number of encounters (Davies 2002). Moreover, sightings are still obtained by including the techniques outlined above, and are more prolonged than brief sightings often obtained during line transects (White and Edwards 2000).

Density estimates

Low density (less than two groups/km2) potentially affects singing frequency, as groups with few neighbours may sing less than groups surrounded by neighbours (Brockelman and Srikosamatara 1993). Our density exceeds this figure, and we witnessed two group encounters involving four groups, indicating that, even if group ranges are not overlapping (Sommer and Reichard 2000), gibbons are coming close to each other. Moreover, research shows that for agile gibbons, singing is spontaneous and not stimulated by other groups (Brockelman and Ali 1987).

Individual density of 7.4 animals/km2 was calculated from a mean group size of 3.4 individuals. Previous research on mean group size of H.a. albibarbis in West Kalimantan, Borneo (Mitani 1990), yielded an average group size of 4.1. This figure is higher than the data in this study, but Mitanirsquos research period was longer and the density is higher in that region. It is possible that some individuals of each group sighted were missed during our study, as the gibbons at Setia Alam are not habituated. In addition, agile gibbon group members can be up to 30 m apart at any given time (Gittins and Raemaekers 1980). Because mean sustained sighting length was 21.2 min, mean group size generated by these data were used in the density analysis, as opposed to referring to published data, as no evidence suggests that average group size is consistent across study sites. In fact, there is large variation regarding density, group size and range size between sites, even if only H. agilis is considered (Table 3).
Table 3 Comparison of densities and home range size for Hylobates agilis across study sites

Location of study site

Taxa

Average group density/km2

No. of study groups

Animal density/km2

Mean group size

Home range (ha)

Habitat type

Reference

LAHG, Central Kalimantan, Indonesia

H.a. albibarbis

2.2

19

7.5

3.4

45

Peat-swamp forest

This study

Gunung Palung Reserve, West Kalimantan, Indonesia

H.a. albibarbis

3.6

10

14.9

4.1

28

Mountain forests. Also beach and mangrove forests, peat- and freshwater-swamp forests and lowland rainforests

Mitani (1990)

Tanjung Puting National Park, South Kalimantan, Indonesia

H.a. albibarbis

–

-

8.7 (n=16)

–

–

Primarily heath forest, also Nypa forest, mangrove and peat-swamp forest

Mather (1992)

Barito Ulu Research Area,Central Kalimantan, Indonesia

H. agilis Χ H. muelleri

2.1

?

8.2

4

43–46

Lowland dipterocarp and heath forest

McConkey et al. (2002)

Bukit Barisan Selatan National Park, Southwest Sumatra, Indonesia

H. agilis agilis

0.7

?

1.4–2.2

2.61

–

70% dipterocarp, forest, also beach forest, freshwater-swamp forest, Nypa forest and hill and mountain forest

OrsquoBrien et al. (2004)

Sungai Dal, Gunung Bubu Forest Reserve, West Malaysia

H. agilis unko

4.3

7

18.9

4.4

29

Lowland and hill dipterocarp forest

Gittins and Raemaekers (1980), Gittins (1982)

Extrapolation of density estimate

This study was conducted within MSF, which is the largest habitat sub-type in the Sabangau catchment, covering 2,622 km2. Extrapolation of the results across this habitat sub-type yields a gibbon population of 19,500 (±4,200) individuals. Besides MSF, two other major habitat sub-types are recorded in the Sabangau catchment; tall interior forest (TIF: 909 km2) and low pole forest (LPF: 1,782 km2). TIF supports the highest diversity of plants and animals in the catchment (Page et al. 1997) and the highest density of orang-utans (Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2003). Gibbons are frequently heard and seen there (S.J. Husson, personal observation). Thus, it can be expected that gibbon density in this habitat sub-type is at least equivalent to that recorded in MSF (and probably higher). This adds an additional 1,960±400 groups, or 6,760±1,445 individuals to the overall total. LPF, by contrast, has a much lower species diversity and orang-utan density than MSF (Page et al. 1997) and gibbons are very rarely heard calling there. For the purposes of this extrapolation it is safest to assume a density of zero. Thus, we estimate an overall population estimate of about 26,300±5,600 individuals. This is a very broad extrapolation, however, and factors such as tree-species composition and nutrient availability are likely to affect local density. Additionally, logging activities have occurred throughout the catchment causing varying levels of damage.

Some studies show gibbons to be fairly tolerant of logging activities (Chivers 1986), particularly in the short-term (Howell 2003, cited in Meijaard et al. 2005) due to the strong territoriality of these species (Wilson and Johns 1982). Other studies, however, show decreases in density of 20–60% between logged and primary forest (e.g. Johns 1992), and others even greater decreases of over 60% (Nijman 1997, Howell 2003, both cited in Meijaard et al. 2005) owing to reductions in food availability. Logging damage is not consistent across the catchment and some areas of forest have been damaged more heavily than at LAHG. We do not know to what degree the gibbon population has suffered in these areas. For these reasons the extrapolation should be treated with caution. Nevertheless, because we probably underestimate gibbon density in the TIF and LPF habitat types, and as solitary individuals are excluded from the analysis, this population estimate may be considered a minimum and is thus useful for conservation planning for H. agilis.

Conservation implications for H.a. albibarbis

IUCN stresses the importance of re-evaluating taxa at regular intervals, particularly those that are listed as lsquoNear Threatenedrsquo or lsquoConservation Dependentrsquo. IUCN last assessed H.a. albibarbis in 2000 (Eudey 2000). In 1977, Chivers estimated the population of H. agilis to be in the region of 744,000 individuals. He attributed 455,000 of these individuals to H.a. albibarbis. His predictions for the speciesrsquo future were bleak, with a 95% loss of H.a. albibarbis over the following 15 years. In 1984, he updated this status, suggesting H.a. albibarbis was lsquowidespreadrsquo and lsquorelatively safersquo (Brockelman and Chivers 1984, p.4). In 1986, MacKinnon published slightly higher population estimates for H. agilis, in the region of 850,000, but did not differentiate between the subspecies of H. agilis. Both studies warn of the crudeness and possible inaccuracy of the data (Chivers 1977; MacKinnon 1986).

Reliable population estimates are confounded by the fact that, with the exception of North Vietnam, gibbon distribution and abundance in Borneo is the most poorly documented (Brandon-Jones et al. 2004). Moreover, what has been documented shows a coastal bias in terms of information (Marshall and Sugardjito 1986; Brandon-Jones et al. 2004).

The density figures in this study are encouraging and indicate that peat-swamp forest is an important habitat for gibbons. At the time of this study, illegal logging presented the greatest threat. Since this study was conducted, the Sabangau catchment has been designated as a national park. With this status in place, Husson et al. (2006) identify, as an immediate conversation priority, the damming of illegal logging extraction canals in an attempt to restore the hydrology of peat-swamp forest within the Sabangau.

The slow life history of gibbons makes them particularly susceptible to population crashes, which is a concern, as much of the habitat occupied by gibbons continues to be destroyed, which could affect survival rates of dispersing individuals (Mitani 1990). Monitoring over time would assess the stability of the Sabangau gibbon population. Whether the newly protected status of the area will have a positive impact on gibbon populations can be considered in future studies. Research should extend the survey area to establish whether there are differences in gibbon density between areas of differing logging damage and habitat sub-types, in order to produce a more accurate estimate of total population size in the Sabangau catchment.

People are slow to realise that large tracts of forest are essential for human survival (Chivers 1977). The environmental benefits and importance of peat-swamp forest have been well documented (MacKinnon et al. 1997a; Page et al. 1997, Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2003). This area could represent one of the largest continuous populations of H.a. albibarbis in Borneo. It is hoped that these data will encourage longitudinal studies of gibbons in the Sabangau catchment.

In conclusion, the major outcomes of this project are as follows:
1.  Hylobates agilis albibarbis are estimated at a density of 2.16 groups/km2 within the Sabangau, a density falling in the mid-range of estimates for agile gibbons across study sites.
2.  Extrapolation of results indicates a potential population of 19,500 individuals (MSF).
3.  Calling times were shown to correlate with rainfall during the night preceding data collection.
4.  Auditory sampling methods can be used to survey gibbons in level forest with adjustments: reduce the effective listening area, and select three listening posts for each sample period.
5.  Probability of calling rates indicates that 3-day sample periods would be sufficient.
6.  Wind level, temperature, rainfall and cloud cover should be recorded during data collection.
Acknowledgements  We thank the management and staff of the Centre for International Cooperation in Management of Tropical Peatland (CIMTROP) and the University of Palangkaraya for permission to work at the LAHG and for logistical assistance throughout the course of this study. We are grateful to the Orang-utan Foundation UK for the provision of a research grant to C.B. Special thanks to research team leaders, Sampang Gaman and Lucie Janssens, and research assistants, Vicky Martin, Gemma Strickland, Rosalie Dench, Richard Hoolahan, Grace Blackham, and Philip McCurley. Also thank you to Suwido Limin, Helen Morrogh-Bernard, Susan Cheyne, Laura DrsquoArcy, Anna Lyons, Thomas Geissmann, David Thurling and Simon Bearder. We are indebted to the reviewers, Warren Brockelman and David Chivers, and the editor Toshisada Nishida, for their useful comments, which have greatly improved this paper.


References

Alfred JRB, Sati JP (1990) Survey and census of the hoolock gibbon in West Garo Hills, Northeast India. Primates 31:299–306
SpringerLink
 
Brandon-Jones D, Eudey AA, Geissmann T, Groves CP, Melnick DJ, Morales JC, Shekelle M, Stewart CB (2004) Asian primate classification. Int J Primatol 25:97–164
SpringerLink
 
Brockelman WY, Ali R (1987) Methods of surveying and sampling forest primate populations. In: Marsh CW, Mittermeier RA (eds) Primate conservation in the tropical rain forest. Liss, New York, pp 23–62
 
Brockelman WY, Chivers DJ (1984) Gibbon conservation: looking to the future. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N (eds) The lesser apes: evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 3–12
 
Brockelman WY, Srikosamatara S (1993) Estimating density of gibbon groups by use of the loud songs. Am J Primatol 29:93–108
CrossRef
 
Chivers DJ (1977) The lesser apes. In: Prince Rainier HSH III, Bourne GH (eds) Primate conservation. Academic, New York, pp 539–598
 
Chivers DJ (1984) Feeding and ranging in gibbons: a summary. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, creel N (eds)The lesser apes: evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 267–284
 
Chivers DJ (1986) Southeast Asian primates. In: Benirsche K (ed) Primates (the road to self-sustaining populations). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 99–126
 
Davies G (2002) Primates. In: Davies G (ed) African forest biodiversity. Earthwatch Press, pp 99–116
 
Estrada A, Castellanos L, Garcia Y, Franco B, Munoz D, Ibarra A, Rivera A, Fuentes E, Jimenez C (2002) Survey of the Black Howler Monkey, Alouatta Pigra, population at the Mayan Site of Palenque, Chiapas, Mexico. Primates 43:51–58
PubMed
 
Estrada A, Luecke L, Van Belle S, Barrueta E, Meda MR (2004) Survey of Black Howler (Alouatta pigra) and spider (Ateles geoffroyi) monkeys in the Mayan Sites of Calakmul and Yaxchilan, Mexico and Tikal, Guatemala. Primates 45:33–39
SpringerLinkPubMed
 
Eudey A and Members of the Primate Specialist Group (2000) Hylobates agilis ssp. albibarbis. In: 2003 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species, IUCN 2003. Retrieved on 9 September from the World Wide Web: http://www.redlist.org/
 
Geissmann T (2000) Retrieved on 1 April 2004 from the World Wide Web: http://www.tiho-hannover.de/gibbons/main2/index.html
 
Geissmann T (2002–2003) Symposium on gibbon diversity and conservation: concluding resolution. Asian Primates 8:28–29
 
Gittins SP (1980) Territorial behaviour in the gibbon. Int J Primatol 1:381–399
 
Gittins SP (1982) Feeding and ranging in the agile gibbon. Folia Primatol 38:39–71
PubMed
 
Gittins SP (1984) The vocal repertoire and song of the agile gibbon. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N (eds) The lesser apes: evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 354–375
 
Gittins SP, Raemaekers JJ (1980) Siamang, lar, and agile gibbons. In: Chivers DJ (ed) Malayan forest primates: ten yearsrsquo study in tropical rain forests. Plenum, New York, pp 63–105
 
Gonzalez-Kirchner JP (1998) Group size and population density of the Black Howler Monkey (Alouatta pigra) in Muchukux Forest, Quintana Roo, Mexico. Folia Primatol 69:260–265
CrossRefPubMed
 
Gursky S (1998) Conservation status of the spectral tarsier Tarsius spectrum: population density and home range size. Folia Primatol 69:191–203
CrossRef
 
Haimoff EH, Yang X, He S, Chen N (1986) Census and survey of wild black-crested gibbons (Hylobates concolor concolor) in Yunnan Province, Peoplersquos Republic of China. Folia Primatol 46:205–214
PubMed
 
Hanya G, Yoshihiro S, Zamma K, Kubo R, Takahata Y (2003) New methods to census primate groups: estimating group density of Japanese macaques by point census. Am J Primatol 60:43–56
CrossRefPubMed
 
Husson SJ, McLardy CS, Morrogh-Bernard HC, DrsquoArcy LJ, Limin SH (2006) The compression effect and orang-utan population decline in the Sebangau peat swamp forest 1996–2003. Biol Conserv (in press)
 
Johns AD (1992) Species conservation in managed forests. In: Whitmore TC, Sayers AJ (eds) Tropical deforestation and species extinction. Chapman and Hall, London, pp 15–53
 
Johns AD (1985) Differential detectability of primates between primary and selectively logged habitats and implications for population surveys. Am J Primatol 8:31–36
CrossRef
 
Leighton DR (1987) Gibbons: territoriality and monogamy. In: Smuts BB (ed) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 135–145
 
MacKinnon K (1986) The conservation status of nonhuman primate in Indonesia. In: Benirsche K (ed) Primates (the road to self-sustaining populations). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 99–126
 
MacKinnon K, Hatta IG, Halim H, Mangalik A (1997a) Introduction. In: Mackinnon K, Hatta IG, Halim H, Mangalik A (eds) The ecology of Kalimantan. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 1–6
 
MacKinnon K, Hatta IG, Halim H, Mangalik A (1997b) Freshwater habitats. In: Mackinnon K, Hatta IG, Halim H, Mangalik A (eds) The ecology of Kalimantan. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 117–131
 
Marshall JT, Sugardjito J (1986) Gibbon systematics. In: Swindler DR, Erwin J (eds) Comparative primate biology, vol 1: systematics, evolution and anatomy. Liss, New York, pp 137–185
 
Mather R (1992) Distribution and abundance of primates in Northern Kalimantan Tengah: comparisons with other parts of Borneo and Peninsular Malaysia. In: Ismail G, Mohamed M, Omar S (eds) Forest biology and conservation in Borneo. Sabah Foundation, Kota Kinabalu, pp 175–189
 
McConkey KR, Ario A, Aldy F, Chivers DJ (2003) Influence of forest seasonality on gibbon food choice in the rain forests of Barito Ulu, Central Kalimantan. Int J Primatol 24:19–32
SpringerLink
 
McLardy C (2002) The orang-utan population of the Sebangau tropical peat swamp forest, Central Kalimantan: anthropogenic threats and conservation. Primate Eye 77:7–8
 
Meijaard E, Sheil D, Nasi R, Augeri D, Rosanbaum B, Iskandar D, Setyawati T, Lammertink M, Rachmatika I, Wong A, Soehartono T, Stanley S, OrsquoBrien T (2005) Life after logging: reconciling wildlife forestry and production forestry in Indonesian Borneo. CIFOR, Bogor, Indonesia
 
Mitani JC (1990) Demography of agile gibbons (Hylobates agilis). Int J Primatol 11:409–422
 
Morrogh-Bernard H, Husson S, Page SE, Rieley JO (2003) Population status of the Bornean orang-utan (Pongo pygmaeus) in the Sebangau peat swamp forest, Central Kalimantan, Indonesia. Biol Conserv 110:141–152
CrossRef
 
National Research Council (1981) Techniques for the study of primate population ecology, National Academy Press, Washington DC
 
Nijman V (2004) Conservation of the Javan gibbon Hylobates moloch: population estimates, local extinctions, and conservation priorities. Raffles Bull Zool 52:271–280
 
OrsquoBrien TG, Kinnaird MF, Nurcahyo A, Prasetyaningrum M, Iqbal M (2003) Fire, demography and persistence of Siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus: Hylobatidae) in a Sumatran Rainforest. Anim Conserv 6:115–121
CrossRef
 
OrsquoBrien TG, Kinnaird MF, Nurcahyo A, Iqbal M, Rusmanto M (2004) Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a Threatened Sumatran Rain Forest. Int J Primatol 25:267–284
SpringerLink
 
Page SE, Rieley JO, Doody K, Hodgson S, Husson S, Jenkins P, Morrogh-Bernard H, Otway S, Wilshaw S (1997) Biodiversity of tropical peat swamp forest: a case study of animal diversity in the Sungai Sebangau catchment of Central Kalimantan, Indonesia. In: Rieley JO, Page SE (eds) Tropical peatlands. Samara, Cardigan, pp 231–242
 
Page SE, Rieley JO, Shotyk OW, Weiss D (1999) Interdependence of peat and vegetation in a tropical peat swamp forest. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 354:1885–1897
CrossRef
 
Palombit R (1994) Dynamic pair bonds in hylobatids: implications regarding monogamous social systems. Behaviour 128:65–101
 
Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM (1985) Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar): territorial, sex-specific and species-specific responses. Anim Behav 33:481–493
CrossRef
 
Reichard U, Sommer V (1997) Group encounters in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar): agonism, affiliation, and the concept of infanticide. Behaviour 134:1135–1174
 
Rieley JO, Page SE, Limin SH, Winarti S (1997) The Peatland resource of Indonesia and the Kalimantan peat swamp forest research project. In: Rieley JO, Page SE (eds) Tropical Peatlands. Samara, Cardigan, pp 37–44
 
Ross C, Reeve N (2003) Survey and census methods: population distribution and density. In: Setchell JM, Curtis DJ (eds) Field and laboratory methods in primatology. a practical guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 90–109
 
Singleton I, Wich S, Husson S, Stephens S, Utami-Atmoko S, Leighton M, Rosen N, Traylor-Holzer K, Lacy R, Byers O (eds) (2004) Orang-utan population and habitat viability assessment: final report. IUCN/SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group, Apple Valley, MN, USA
 
Sommer V, Reichard U (2000) Rethinking monogamy: the gibbon case. In: Kappeler P (ed) Primate males. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 159–168
 
Sutherland WJ (2000) Monitoring mammals. In: Sutherland WJ (ed) The conservation handbook: research, management and policy. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 57–59
 
Sutherland WJ (2002) Mammals. In: Sutherland WJ (ed) Ecological census techniques. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 260–278
 
White L, Edwards A (2000) Conservation research in the African rain forests. The Wildlife Conservation Society, New York
 
Wilson WL, Johns AD (1982) Diversity and abundance of selected animal species in undisturbed forest, selectively logged forest and plantations in East Kalimantan, Indonesia. Biol Conserv 24:205–218
CrossRef